NASA would like you to take a picture of a tree, please. The space agency’s ICESat-2 satellite estimates the height of trees from space, and NASA has created a new tool for citizen scientists that can help check those measurements from the ground. All it takes is a smartphone, the app, an optional tape measure, and a tree. So to help, the Verge Science video team went on a mission to measure some massive trees in California as accurately as they can.
Launched in September 2018, the ICESat-2 satellite carries an instrument called ATLAS that shoots 60,000 pulses of light at the Earth’s surface every second it orbits the planet. “It’s basically a laser in space,” says Tom Neumann, the project scientist for ICESat-2 at NASA Goddard Space Flight Center. By measuring the satellite’s position, the angle, and how long it takes for those laser beams to bounce back from the surface, scientists can measure the elevation of sea ice, land ice, the ocean, inland water, and trees. Knowing how tall trees are can help researchers estimate the health of the world’s forests and the amount of carbon dioxide they can soak up.
But Neumann says that a big open question is how good those measurements from space actually are. That’s where the citizen scientist comes in — to help verify them. Some are more challenging than others. “You can’t really ask a bunch of school kids in Pennsylvania to go to Antarctica to measure the ice sheet height for you for a calibration,” he says. But you can ask them to take their smartphones outside, which is exactly what NASA is doing with its GLOBE Observer app. “You’ve got all sorts of great terrain and features right in your backyard that you could go out and do these measurements that would be useful for us,” Neumann says.
The NASA GLOBE Observer app is pretty user-friendly. After you download it, you can choose from different tools that record cloud observations, mosquito habitats, and the landscape around you. There’s also a new tool for measuring trees, called GLOBE Trees. When you first open it, a tutorial walks you through how to calibrate the app and take the measurements that let it triangulate tree heights. The tutorial includes helpful tips for things like “Selecting a tree” — apparently, bent and broken ones don’t get measured.
Once you’ve selected your non-broken tree and staked out a spot about 25 to 75 feet away, you hold the phone right in front of your face and angle it to measure the base and then the tree’s top. Then you take a picture, count your steps to the tree, log your position at its base, and the app spits out the tree’s height. Verge Science’s Cory Zapatka and Will Poor took the app for a test drive in Central Park and across the country in Northern California, home to some of the tallest trees in the world. They checked the app’s math by trying some of the measurements manually with a tape measure and an angle-measuring tool called a clinometer. To see how that turned out, check out the video.
Since the official launch at the end of March, GLOBE Trees has received about 700 measurements from around 20 different countries, according to senior NASA Earth Science outreach specialist Brian Campbell, the Trees science lead. And the researchers would love to get even more. The measurements are useful data for the ICESat-2 team, Neumann says. So when it comes to people using the app, he says, “The more, the merrier.”
Update May 7, 2019 10:00 AM ET: This story was originally published at 10:44AM ET on April 15, 2019. It has been updated to include details about Verge Science’s new video.
]]>Through both sunny days and torrential storms, sailors cutting through the waters around New Zealand and Antarctica faithfully recorded the weather they encountered, building up a treasure trove of data. Over a century later, scientists are digging through these maritime records for insights about the past and future of the region’s climate — and they need the public’s help.
Knowing what the weather was up to in the past can help scientists calibrate climate models like the ones they use to predict how weather conditions are likely to change as global temperatures continue to rise. Sailors traveling around New Zealand from the late 1800s to the mid-1900s to trade goods, hunt whales, or explore Antarctica kept logs about the water and air temperatures, air pressure, sea ice, and wind. “Those logbooks are an absolutely massive source of weather data that we can use to improve our historical record of what we know about New Zealand’s climate and the climate of the surrounding oceans,” says Petra Pearce, a climate scientist with New Zealand’s National Institute of Water and Atmospheric Research (NIWA).
“Those logbooks are an absolutely massive source of weather data.”
The trouble is that these logbooks are handwritten, which means they’re hard for humans to read and even harder for a computer make out. So Pearce and a team of researchers turned to the public for help transcribing these logbooks. The project is called Southern Weather Discovery, and it launched in October 2018. Since then, volunteers have transcribed more than 200 logbooks from early 20th century merchant ships, Pearce says. “People do really want to be involved in science.”
The website says they’re around 89 percent of the way through the transcriptions, but Pearce says not to be fooled by that stat. This is a long-term project, and there are many more logbooks to photograph, process, and upload for people to analyze. Next on the list are logs from whaling vessels, which Pearce is particularly excited about because those ships went right down to Antarctica. “We don’t have really any data for Antarctica because, obviously, there was no one there 100 years ago, apart from the odd exploration,” she says.
The Verge spoke with Pearce about bad handwriting, the aurora, and expeditions that never made it back from Antarctica.
This interview has been lightly edited for clarity and brevity.
What’s your favorite thing that you’ve learned during this process?
One of the most interesting things was reading the logbooks from Robert Falcon Scott’s 1911 to 1912 expedition to the South Pole. Scott was part of the British Antarctic expedition, and they sailed down there in a race for the South Pole. They got beaten by the Norwegians, and on their way back from the South Pole, tragically, Scott and the few of his men that were with him at the time died from exposure. But they were taking measurements of temperature and barometric pressure and things like that right up until they died on the ice. You could see their observations getting more and more sporadic over the last days of their journey before they died, and it was very humbling to think they kept going, even in the face of horrible circumstances. They kept going and kept taking these observations. They didn’t know what would happen with them. And to think that over 100 years later, we’re now using these observations for research to understand what was going on and what may happen in the future — that’s huge.
Why are you asking people to transcribe weather data in logbook entries?
All of these logbooks that we have are handwritten, so they are very difficult to have read by a computer. Technology’s not at a point yet where that can be automated, although the research is underway into how we can automate reading that handwriting. You can have the computer give you what it thinks these handwritten numbers are. But you need to trust that, you need to know that that’s right. And we haven’t gotten to the point where we trust that completely yet, so it’s an ongoing process.
“It’s a really big human effort.”
At the moment, it’s a really big human effort, and that’s why we started Southern Weather Discovery — to get people from around the world involved. They can basically help us to transcribe this weather data that we really need to undertake our research. We have this big database of all of our climate data from these logbooks — and other sources as well — that we can basically put into climate models to refine the image of that weather map going back in time.
How does the transcription process work?
We have this logbook page, and it’s got a lot of information on it, different columns and rows. That’s quite a lot for someone to look at. They’ll say, “Oh, I can’t transcribe that. That’s way too much.” So we crop little sections of these logbook pages out, put them onto the website, and someone can come onto the website and type in the numbers they see. Sometimes the numbers are a little bit difficult to decipher. It’s really old-style, curly handwriting. So we have some help guides to say, “Does your number look like this? It might be a 7, or it might be a 4.”
We also have 10 different people transcribe the same image for quality control. That way, if someone types in a wrong number, that wrong number doesn’t get propagated through to the global database. People can undertake as little or as much as they want. We’ve got people who have come in, and they’ve done just a little bit, and they’re like, “Okay cool, that’s not for me.” But there are other people who, even six months later, are regularly taking part, and that’s really cool to see.
What do you do with that data once people transcribe it?
We download a huge amount of data from the website and go through a quality control process to weed out the incorrect data that people may have keyed. Then we put it all back together, and we eventually upload those now-typed logbook data into global databases. They will directly go into improving the data coverage for this part of the world and helping us to understand what was going on in terms of climate back in the day.
We’re still sifting through a lot of the data that’s been keyed so far. There have been some pretty interesting things we’ve seen looking at some of the notes in there — like observations of icebergs quite far north in the warmer waters where you wouldn’t normally experience them. Observations of wildlife and whales, the aurora (the southern lights), and absolutely horrible storms that these ships were traveling through. You can imagine a lot of them were steamships, but a lot of them were also sailing ships that were fighting through some horrendous conditions and still taking their observations religiously multiple times a day.
How close are you to being finished?
It’s a moving target. We’ve had a lot of data transcribed already on the website. But behind the scenes, we are still processing the logbooks, and we upload new sets of data every so often. It’s a long-term project. It’s not just something that’s going to end when we finish the data that we’ve got on there at the moment. We would love people to come and help. If people have these old logbooks in their houses, that’s also something that’s really valuable. If they wanted to let us know they have that and we can do something with that data, that’s amazing as well. We’ve had a couple of people actually call us up and say, “Hey! I have all this data from 100 years ago — do you want it?” And we’re like, “Yes, thank you! That’d be great!” It’s really cool some of the things that come out of the woodwork. And it’s really cool to engage with the public in this process rather than just publish a scientific article about it. We’re actually engaging people and helping them to understand why we do what we do.
]]>As the number of measles cases in the United States has surged, some officials are encouraging parents to get their children vaccinated sooner rather than later. Normally, kids would get their first measles, mumps, and rubella vaccine when they’re about a year old, but given the rise in cases, babies in areas with ongoing measles outbreaks may be able to get theirs earlier in an effort to keep some of the youngest and most vulnerable members of those communities safe.
The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention reported on Monday that the number of people infected with measles in the US continues to climb, reaching 704 as of April 26th. That’s the highest case count since 1994, and 2019 still has eight months remaining for that number to rise. The CDC blames misinformation about vaccines for driving undervaccination, which has allowed measles to spread in the recent outbreaks. The vast majority of cases in this most recent outbreak were linked to communities with low vaccination rates.
Young children, babies, pregnant women, and people with weakened immune systems are especially at risk of suffering complications from the incredibly contagious virus. That’s why health officials are doing everything they can to encourage people in areas with active measles outbreaks to get vaccinated, including — in some places — lowering the recommended vaccination age.
“Vaccines are safe. Vaccines do not cause autism. Vaccine-preventable diseases are dangerous.”
In New York, which has seen hundreds of measles cases since last fall, the state’s Department of Health has given doctors the go-ahead to lower the vaccination age to six months in areas with ongoing outbreaks, according to Erin Silk, a spokesperson for the department. New York City Mayor Bill de Blasio has ordered that everyone — including babies as young as six months old — get their measles vaccinations. The measles vaccine is safe and effective, despite the thoroughly debunked myth that vaccines cause autism. An extra measles vaccine at six months is very safe, too, according to Peter Hotez, dean for the National School of Tropical Medicine at Baylor College of Medicine. It just isn’t the norm because, under typical, non-outbreak circumstances, the vaccine may not be as effective for babies that young. “However, some babies could still benefit from early immunization during a true measles outbreak,” he says in an email to The Verge.
Vaccines can help protect people against infections by essentially giving the immune system a training bout against a weaker opponent. The MMR vaccine, for example, contains weakened versions of the live viruses. By fighting them off, the immune system develops a memory of how to respond if these viruses actually attack at full strength in the future. “The goal is to induce an immune response that’s the product of natural infection, without having to pay the price of natural infection,” says Paul Offit, director of the Vaccine Education Center at the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia.
Babies are born with some passive immunity in the form of antibodies that they receive from their moms. After delivery and before they wear off, those antibodies help protect the infant during its first months. The timing can vary from baby to baby — some are protected longer than six months, others less. But if those antibodies are still around when the baby gets their measles vaccine, they may also glom onto the measles vaccine itself and neutralize it, preventing it from provoking enough of an immune response to protect the baby over the short or long term. That’s why doctors recommend the first dose of measles vaccine when they do. But in outbreaks, that can change.
Some added protection may be necessary in outbreak situations where lots of babies could be exposed. Public health authorities might recommend adding an earlier dose for babies as young as six months old, according to Kristen Nordlund, a spokesperson for the CDC. The recommendation is for those kids to then be revaccinated when they’re about a year old and get the next dose when they’re between ages four and six, according to the CDC. Since the measles outbreaks in the US originated with people who visited other countries, the CDC also recommends in its recent measles update that kids older than six months get one dose of the MMR, and adults make sure to get their second before traveling outside the US. Hotez suggests that any parents who have questions talk to their kids’ pediatrician.
Making sure everyone who can be vaccinated is vaccinated is the best way to keep the virus from gaining traction in the US again, according to the CDC. As CDC director Robert Redfield said in a statement on Monday: “Vaccines are safe. Vaccines do not cause autism. Vaccine-preventable diseases are dangerous.”
]]>The National Academy of Sciences took a major step today to oust sexual harassers when members at its annual meeting voted to approve a new amendment that would allow the organization to kick out people who badly violate its new code of conduct. This vote isn’t the final verdict, however: the entire membership of the NAS still needs to weigh in — a process that’s expected to be wrapped up by mid-June.
The National Academy of Sciences, or NAS, was founded in 1863, and is one of the three academies that make up the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. Together, they conduct massive research analyses and produce reports to help “solve complex problems and inform public policy decisions,” according to their website. One of these, published last year, reported that women in scientific, engineering, and medical fields face rampant sexual harassment in academia.
According to Nature, this amendment is only for the National Academy of Sciences. And it comes almost a year after neuroscientist BethAnn McLaughlin started an online petition to yank membership from NAS members “who have been sanctioned for sexual harassment, retaliation and assault.”
The new amendment, if it’s finalized, could help the National Academy of Sciences deal with proven sexual harassers in its ranks. There have been multiple, according to Science Magazine. The list includes Geoffrey Marcy at the University of California Berkeley, “who was found to have violated campus sexual harassment policies between 2001 and 2010,” Buzzfeed News revealed in 2015. The amendment would also cover other forms of misconduct outlined in the National Academy of Sciences code, such as falsifying scientific data, plagiarizing, bullying, and discriminating.
]]>It feels intuitive to compare caffeine and nicotine, two drugs made by plants that can produce a bit of a buzz. But intuition isn’t the only thing at the root of that association: a concerted public relations effort by Big Tobacco has helped make it stick.
Industry documents, court testimony, and advertisements all show that the tobacco industry has been working for decades to equate nicotine with innocuous vices like coffee, tea, or gummy bears. The effort aims to erode damaging connections to substances with worse reputations, like heroin or cocaine. Now, that same caffeine-nicotine connection is flavoring the discourse about electronic cigarettes, too. “Chemically, nicotine is very similar to caffeine, and coffee is one of the most widely traded products in the world,” Juul co-founder James Monsees told The Mercury News in September 2018. “While people of all ages around the world enjoy coffee, nicotine has been heavily stigmatized.”
But nicotine and caffeine aren’t chemically similar, not really. When I covered the health effects of nicotine last year, I asked Adam Leventhal, director of the USC Health, Emotion, and Addiction Laboratory, how the two drugs compared. “They’re apples and oranges,” he told me, citing a long list of distressing nicotine withdrawal symptoms like anxiety, depression, irritability, and hunger. Clinical pharmacologist Neal Benowitz, a professor at the University of California, San Francisco, says the same thing.
There is a link, in that smoking cigarettes may speed up the body’s metabolism of caffeine, so smokers can wind up drinking more coffee to maintain their caffeine buzz, Benowitz says. Nicotine may also boost the rewarding effects of other substances, like coffee, he says, “which means coffee might actually taste better to someone using nicotine.” So there’s a perception that coffee and cigarettes pair well. “And that’s what I think leads people to connect them,” Benowitz says.
Still, they’re very different drugs. Caffeine is a stimulant that works primarily by blocking the receptor for adenosine, a neurotransmitter that causes relaxation, Benowitz says. That can increase adrenaline and boost blood pressure. Nicotine, by contrast, can increase the release of multiple different neurotransmitters. That’s why for the people who use it, nicotine can act both as a stimulant and a relaxant in different contexts. “People can experience so many different things from nicotine depending on the situation in which they take it,” says Benowitz, who is not funded by any tobacco or e-cigarette companies, but who has consulted for pharmaceutical companies about smoking cessation medications. “It’s no accident that people become addicted,” he says.
That’s another place where nicotine and caffeine differ: the vast majority of people don’t become addicted to caffeine, says Benowitz, who describes addiction as “compulsive use and loss of control over drug use.” If a doctor tells someone to switch to decaf, most people can, despite the headache. But people have a harder time ignoring nicotine cravings when they’re trying to stop smoking, Benowitz says. “There’s much more positive reward and the withdrawal symptoms are more disruptive,” he says. “Nicotine and caffeine are different.”
So if nicotine and caffeine really are apples and oranges, why do they show up together so often? Robert Jackler, a professor at Stanford University who studies tobacco marketing, has found ads going back to at least 1913 showing people smoking cigarettes while drinking coffee. Jackler thinks that the juxtaposition of cigarettes and coffee was part of a marketing push to make these tubes of tobacco leaves look essential to daily life. “One of the lead messages was that smoking adds pleasure to anything pleasurable you do — whether it’s romance or meals or coffee or liquor or fun at the beach,” Jackler says.
That strategy gained steam after the 1988 US Surgeon General’s report that called out nicotine as the drug responsible for addiction to cigarettes and other tobacco products, according to Pamela Ling, a professor at UCSF who studies tobacco industry marketing strategies. The worst part — at least, from an industry perspective — was that the Surgeon General said, “The pharmacologic and behavioral processes that determine tobacco addiction are similar to those that determine addiction to drugs such as heroin and cocaine.”
“Caffeine use is socially accepted — might enhance social acceptance of nicotine.”
Those weren’t bedfellows that the tobacco industry wanted. “And so they started this big campaign in the ‘80s to say, ‘No, nicotine is not like heroin or cocaine. It’s like caffeine or chocolate. It’s a benign substance,’” Ling says. That campaign included a concerted public relations push. By analyzing internal documents from tobacco company RJ Reynolds, Ling discovered that their communications strategy encouraged caffeine analogies. “Caffeine use is socially accepted — might enhance social acceptance of nicotine,” a 1993 RJ Reynolds document says.
Some of these analogies were disseminated through an industry-funded proxy called Associates for Research in the Science of Enjoyment (ARISE), according to a 2007 study published in the European Journal of Public Health by tobacco researcher Elizabeth Smith. Smith, an adjunct professor at UCSF, dug through news stories and internal tobacco company documents looking for references to ARISE. Created after that 1988 report by the US Surgeon General, Smith’s paper says, “ARISE’s message was that ‘a little of what you fancy can do you good.’”
Under the guise of promoting “objectivity” and “debate,” ARISE sowed doubt about the addictiveness and harm of smoking through conferences and opinion polls. These conferences, Smith says, gave the industry an excuse to put out grabby press releases about tobacco as pleasurable rather than addictive. “It provided a way for the industry to get journalists to tell this kind of story and spread this idea that tobacco isn’t as bad for you as everyone’s been saying,” she says. “The fact that it’s complete bullshit doesn’t really matter in the long run.”
“It’s complete bullshit.”
Of the hundreds of articles Smith uncovered that mentioned ARISE between 1989 and 2005‚ 483 explicitly mentioned tobacco. And of those, more than 94 percent also mentioned chocolate, coffee, tea, and alcohol. The common refrain from ARISE members quoted in news articles was that a “favourite treat, such as a cup of tea or coffee, a glass of wine or beer, a cigarette or a bar of chocolate, reduces stress and helps people relax.” This minimizes the health risks of cigarette addiction and instead frames smoking as an occasional, potentially beneficial, indulgence. The trouble is, Smith says, “tobacco isn’t like that.”
It’s an argument that the industry has stuck to again and again as it’s defended itself against lawsuits. One expert witness compared nicotine to caffeine — and also to fingernail chewing — in order to dismiss nicotine addiction as simply a habit, according to a 2007 review of the industry’s legal strategies. Another said, “Nicotine and caffeine are fundamentally different from addicting drugs like heroin and cocaine.” The repetition of the claim doesn’t make it true — but it does help it stick.
The rise of the e-cigarette industry has added another layer of complexity to the situation. The industry separates nicotine from tobacco to make its products, even as it becomes more enmeshed with the tobacco industry itself. But even with new ways to deliver nicotine, the e-cigarette business still appears to be using the same playbook to cast nicotine as the misunderstood chemical sibling of caffeine. Jackler has discovered that the link shows up subtly in industry advertisements that pair e-cigarettes and coffee in the same frame. Coffee-flavored vape juice adds a new, strange connection to the mix.
Jackler thinks that part of this juxtaposition is a callback to those traditional tobacco industry advertisements that focus on the pleasure that smoking adds to everyday moments, like having a cup of coffee. When the e-cigarette industry does it, he says, “It’s to associate vaping with pleasurable moments in the day.”
Juul has since pulled their ads from social media, and they say they’re careful to toe the line when it comes to alerting people about nicotine in their products. “JUULpods have always indicated that they contain nicotine,” a spokesperson told The Verge in an email. “When FDA required all e-cigarette manufacturers to place additional warnings on its packaging and in its advertising, JUUL Labs promptly complied with those requirements.” And British American Tobacco, the company behind the Vype brand of vapes that has also combined coffee and vape imagery on social media, says: “We know that there could be certain points in the day that some of our consumers will want to enjoy one of our products — and vaping whilst having a coffee is one such example of this. This is factually reflected in the imagery. To be clear, this is not about comparing nicotine to caffeine.”
“Come on.”
But vaping heavy-hitters have made the caffeine-nicotine link explicitly, too — like when Juul’s co-founder bemoaned the stigmatization of nicotine compared to caffeine in his conversation with The Mercury News. In its policy position, an advocacy group called the Smoke-Free Alternatives Trade Association, or SFATA, calls nicotine, “a naturally occurring and relatively harmless substance that has roughly the equivalent danger to the individual’s health as caffeine.” The claims echo former NJOY CEO Craig Weiss, who said in a 2013 CNBC appearance: “Pharmacologically, it’s very similar to caffeine. It’s a stimulant that provides energy but in the case of nicotine, it’s also a relaxing and calming effect.” (The anchor pushed back: “As a smoker, I adore the fact that you’re prepared to say that, actually, nicotine is better for you than caffeine. Come on.”)
Mark Anton, executive director of SFATA says there’s more to the argument than whether nicotine is more like caffeine or heroin — especially when it’s decoupled from cigarette smoke. “There are scientists on both sides of the aisle: one says it’s as addictive as heroin, and others say it’s like caffeine,” he says. “The cigarette companies were trying, as you say, to minimize their exposure, their risk and keep people using those products,” Anton tells me. “And then on the other side we have people that don’t want people to consume nicotine at all, because [as they say] it’s really just bad. But we don’t really know how bad it is because it’s never really been decoupled except by NRT products, and NRT products are considered to be safe and effective.” NRT stands for nicotine replacement therapy, which, according to the Food and Drug Administration, includes nicotine gum, patches, and lozenges. It doesn’t include e-cigarettes or vapes, which the FDA says “would need to be proven safe and effective for smoking cessation and regulated as a drug product.”
Jackler and Ling think this push to lump nicotine and caffeine together is part of an ongoing effort to downplay the risks of e-cigarettes. “I think it’s pretty well accepted that the most dangerous way to use nicotine is through smoking cigarettes,” Ling says. Electronic cigarettes are generally considered less risky than the conventional kind, but they’re not risk-free, particularly for non-smokers and young people. Researchers are still investigating the long-term health effects, but different labs have drawn links between e-cigarettes and potential heart problems, lung irritation, wheezing, and, of course, nicotine addiction. There’s also an ongoing debate about the degree to which e-cigarettes really help people quit smoking.
The rise of e-cigarettes led the industry back to this old idea, Ling says. But where trivializing nicotine addiction by comparing it to caffeine was once part of the Big Tobacco’s larger strategy to deny the danger altogether, now it’s part of an effort to promote the idea of “clean nicotine.” “If e-cigarettes are positioned as a safe way to use nicotine, then the e-cigarette companies are going to embrace this idea that nicotine itself isn’t harmful and might be beneficial,” she says. “In the end, both the tobacco industry in the ‘80s and the e-cigarette industry now both benefit from arguing that nicotine addiction is not a big deal.” That means that even as cigarette smoking continues to decline in the US, the rebranded nicotine industry may keep trying to sell this very old story to a new audience — even though there’s nothing to back up the buzz.
Update April 26th, 2019 6:30PM ET: This story has been updated to include a comment from Mark Anton, executive director of SFATA.
]]>Much has been made of the World Health Organization’s new recommendations that caregivers restrict the amount of time young kids stare at screens. But the guidelines are less about the risks of screen time itself, and more about the advantages of spending time doing pretty much anything else.
The recommendations are broadly about physical activity and sleep for children under five years old, and are an attempt to create healthy habits during a critical developmental window. Among the recommendations for tummy time and active play, the WHO also spells out that between the ages of two and five, children should spend no more than an hour a day plopped in front of a screen. And children under the age of two shouldn’t engage in sedentary screen time at all, the WHO says.
So there’s more to these new guidelines than just screen time. “But I think there has been a lot of interest in the sedentary screen time recommendations in particular,” says Juana Willumsen, a WHO advisor on childhood obesity and physical activity. “It’s something that parents, families, and people in general are concerned about.”
Screen time can mean a lot of things: it can mean getting sucked into an endless stream of YouTube videos, watching TV, playing video games, scrolling through social media, or FaceTiming with grandparents. There’s a lot of debate about what all this digital media is doing to people’s — especially children’s — brains. And the truth is that science hasn’t caught up to the worry raging in places like Silicon Valley, where one parent told The New York Times that “the devil lives in our phones and is wreaking havoc on our children.”
In this case, however, the WHO isn’t basing its recommendations on what screen time can do to the brain. “We didn’t specifically look for evidence about the effects of screens, in terms of the light emitted, for example, or the content that’s on the screen a child is watching, and cognitive development,” Willumsen says. “We were specifically looking at sedentary behavior.” So sitting or lying down and watching TV counts against the WHO’s recommended limit; dancing along with the TV doesn’t. Staring at YouTube on a tablet counts; reading along with a parent on an e-reader doesn’t. FaceTiming family is fine, too, Willumsen says.
Those distinctions are key to iron out as researchers continue investigating the effects of screen time, says Marc Potenza, a professor of psychiatry at Yale. “I would argue that not all forms of screen time are the same with respect to their potential beneficial aspects and potential detrimental aspects,” he says. That doesn’t mean the WHO should have held off on issuing recommendations, he says, just that people should be prepared for this guidance to change as we learn more. “There are children growing up now, and parents have questions about how they should raise their children in this environment.”
For Michael Rich, the director of the Center on Media and Child Health at Boston Children’s Hospital, focusing on the WHO’s screen time recommendations misses the big picture: “It’s not that the screen is potentially toxic, per se, it is that it is a relatively impoverished stimulus for them compared to face-to-face interaction,” he says. Screen time, in this context, essentially becomes a marker for how people interact with children — and the important part is giving kids a diverse range of experiences, he says. He’d like to see recommendations for easy alternatives — like listening to music. Otherwise, he says, “Setting a screen time limit probably generates more guilt than enlightenment.”
With these kinds of screen time limits the burden falls on parents and caregivers to follow them. And Jenny Radesky, an assistant professor of pediatrics at the University of Michigan, hopes that in the future, the WHO also makes recommendations to improve children’s digital environment. “These might include less usage before bed or in the overnight hours, healthier content that’s truly educational not just marketed as such, and reducing persuasive features that young minds can’t resist,” she says. “This would put less of the onus on parents to always be the gatekeepers for children’s media behaviors.”
]]>Measles just hit a major milestone as it spreads across the country, infecting the most people since the year 2000, when public health officials declared the virus eliminated in the US. Thanks to the anti-vaccination movement, the virus has come roaring back.
The case count has climbed to 695 people infected across 22 different states, driven in part by outbreaks that have lingered in New York and Washington, according to a statement from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Stopping these ongoing outbreaks as soon as possible will be critical, the CDC says. “The longer these outbreaks continue, the greater the chance measles will again get a sustained foothold in the United States,” the CDC said in a statement.
“The longer these outbreaks continue, the greater the chance measles will again get a sustained foothold”
Measles, recognized for its rash, can also cause pneumonia, brain swelling, and death. The Measles, Mumps, and Rubella — or MMR — vaccine is safe and can ward off measles infections. But some people, like children under one year old and those who can’t be vaccinated for medical reasons, rely on the rest of us being vaccinated to keep the notoriously contagious virus from spreading.
If everyone who can get vaccinated does, outbreaks are small to non-existent, the CDC says. But when someone with measles visits a community that isn’t adequately vaccinated, the outbreak can metastasize. That’s because a sneeze can squirt the virus into the air, where it can stick around for up to two hours. And 90 percent of unvaccinated people who are exposed to someone with the infection will catch it.
The CDC points to inadequate vaccination and the rise of vaccine misinformation as a driver of the New York outbreaks, in particular. “Some organizations are deliberately targeting these communities with inaccurate and misleading information about vaccines,” the CDC says. Vox reported earlier this month that anti-vaxx organizations have directed fear-mongering misinformation about the health risks of vaccines at orthodox Jewish communities in New York.
Alex Azar, secretary of the U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, tried to calm the fearful in a statement today. “The measles vaccines are among the most extensively studied medical products we have, and their safety has been firmly established over many years in some of the largest vaccine studies ever undertaken,” he said. “With a safe and effective vaccine that protects against measles, the suffering we are seeing is avoidable.”
]]>Free apps marketed to people with depression or who want to quit smoking are hemorrhaging user data to third parties like Facebook and Google — but often don’t admit it in their privacy policies, a new study reports. This study is the latest to highlight the potential risks of entrusting sensitive health information to our phones.
Though most of the easily-found depression or smoking cessation apps in the Android and iOS stores share data, only a fraction of them actually disclose this. The findings add to a string of worrying revelations about what apps are doing with the health information we entrust to them. For instance, a Wall Street Journal investigation recently revealed the period tracking app Flo shared users’ period dates and pregnancy plans with Facebook. And previous studies have reported health apps with security flaws or that shared data with advertisers and analytics companies.
In this new study, published Friday in the journal JAMA Network Open, researchers searched for apps using the keywords “depression” and “smoking cessation.” Then they downloaded the apps and checked to see whether the data put into them was shared by intercepting the app’s traffic. Much of the data the apps shared didn’t immediately identify the user or was even strictly medical. But 33 of the 36 apps shared information that could give advertisers or data analytics companies insights into people’s digital behavior. And a few shared very sensitive information, like health diary entries, self reports about substance use, and usernames.
“It’s important to trust but verify.”
Those kinds of details, plus the name or type of app, could give third parties information about someone’s mental health that the person might want to keep private. “Even knowing that a user has a mental health or smoking cessation app downloaded on their phone is valuable ‘health-related’ data,” Quinn Grundy, an assistant professor at the University of Toronto who studies corporate influences on health and was not involved in the study, tells The Verge in an email.
The fact that people might not know how their apps are sharing their data worried John Torous, director of digital psychiatry at Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and a co-author on the new study. “It’s really hard to make an informed decision about using an app if you don’t even know who’s going to get access to some information about you,” he says. That’s why he and a team at the University of New South Wales in Sydney ran this study. “It’s important to trust but verify — to say where is your healthcare data going,” Torous says.
“They’re basically lying.”
By intercepting the data transmissions, they discovered that 92 percent of the 36 apps shared the data with at least one third party — mostly Facebook- and Google-run services that help with marketing, advertising, or data analytics. (Facebook and Google did not immediately respond to requests for comment.) But about half of those apps didn’t disclose that third-party data sharing, for a few different reasons: nine apps didn’t have a privacy policy at all; five apps did but didn’t say the data would be shared this way; and three apps actively said that this kind of data sharing wouldn’t happen. Those last three are the ones that stood out to Steven Chan, a physician at Veterans Affairs Palo Alto Health Care System, who has collaborated with Torous in the past but wasn’t involved in the new study. “They’re basically lying,” he says of the apps.
The researchers don’t know what these third-party sites were doing with this user data. “We live in an age where, with enough breadcrumbs, it’s possible to reidentify people,” Torous says. It’s also possible the breadcrumbs just sit there, he says — but for now, they just don’t know. “What happens to this digital data is kind of a mystery.” But Chan worries about the potential, invisible risks. “Potentially advertisers could use this to compromise someone’s privacy and sway their treatment decisions,” he says. For example, what if an advertiser discovers someone is trying to quit smoking? “Maybe if someone is interested in smoking, would they be interested in electronic cigarettes?” Chan says. “Or could they potentially introduce them to other similar products, like alcohol?”
“What happens to this digital data is kind of a mystery.”
Part of the problem is the business model for free apps, the study authors write: since insurance might not pay for an app that helps users quit smoking, for example, the only ways for free app developer to stay afloat is to either sell subscriptions or sell data. And if that app is branded as a wellness tool, the developers can skirt laws intended to keep medical information private.
So Torous recommends caution before sharing sensitive information with an app. The potential for mental health apps to help people is exciting, Torous says. “But I think it does mean you want to pause twice and say, ‘Do I trust the person who made the app, and do I understand where this data is going?’” A few quick gut checks could include making sure that the app has a privacy policy, that it’s been updated recently, and that the app comes from a trustworthy source like a medical center or the government. “None of those questions are going to guarantee you a good result, but they’re going to probably help you screen,” he says.
Long-term, one way to protect people who want to use health and wellness apps could be to form a group that can give a stamp of approval to responsible mental health apps, Chan says. “Kind of like having the FDA’s approval on things, or the FAA certifying a particular aircraft for safety,” he says. But for now, it’s app-user beware. “When there are no such institutions or the institutions themselves aren’t doing a good job, it means we need to invest more as a public good.”
]]>Editor’s note, April 19th, 2019: We’re republishing this story today because of a report at Quartz that — like the infamous memo from former Google employee James Damore — some Microsoft workers are now internally criticizing the company’s diversity policies, suggesting yet again that biological differences may make women less suited for engineering jobs. This story may help explain why that logic is flawed.
In 2017, the idea that biological differences drive social inequality is considered fairly offensive. For the incurious, the taboo around this argument makes it exciting. But unlike people, not all ideas are created equally, and they should not be treated with the same amount of seriousness — especially when those ideas ignore both a broad scientific debate that’s gone on for years and clear evidence that women in tech are excluded more than in other industries.
The idea that women or people of color lack the innate qualities that white men possess to succeed in high-status, elite professions is decades old. And the shape of the argument always looks the same, saying that current social conditions are somehow biologically natural, and that attempts to remedy inequalities are suspect. It is a tired stance in an endless debate, and it says far more about our feelings than it does about science.
This time, those feelings came from a software engineer named James Damore.
After attending a voluntary diversity training session at Google, Damore penned a 10-page memo entitled “Google’s Ideological Echo Chamber.” In it, Damore reaches for science to explain away the gender gap in tech, arguing that female biology is, in part, holding women back. Programs aimed at boosting diversity in tech are “as misguided and biased as mandating increases for women’s representation in the homeless, work-related and violent deaths, prisons, and school dropouts,” he writes.
The manifesto circulated internally for about a month before going viral, drawing condemnation from some quarters, and approval from others. Damore has since been fired and has filed a labor complaint against Google.
A tired stance in an endless debate
In an all-company memo, Google CEO Sundar Pichai said that while “much of what was in that memo is fair to debate,” Damore went too far. “To suggest a group of our colleagues have traits that make them less biologically suited to that work is offensive and not OK,” Pichai wrote.
Damore pushed back on Friday in a Wall Street Journal op-ed, writing that the document was “a reasoned, well-researched, good-faith argument.” He asks: “How did Google, the company that hires the smartest people in the world, become so ideologically driven and intolerant of scientific debate and reasoned argument?”
The answer is that the debate is old and the arguments are well-worn and generally bad. In 2005, Larry Summers, then-president of Harvard University, suggested at an economics conference that women were underrepresented among faculty scientists because of “innate” differences. That year, only four of 32 tenure offers made by Harvard’s Faculty of Arts and Sciences went to women. In 1994, Charles Murray and Richard Herrnstein published The Bell Curve, which focuses on race and IQ, kicking off a controversy that rages to this day. We have been having this argument forever.
Damore’s memo is sloppy and fundamentally unscientific
In this context, Damore’s memo is particularly sloppy and fundamentally unscientific. The memo doesn’t clearly define what makes for a successful coder. It doesn’t explore what scientists do and don’t know about how biological sex shapes behavior. It doesn’t call on experts to debate. It doesn’t evaluate the strengths and weaknesses of the evidence. And it never once discusses what we know about gender discrimination or its long, sordid history in tech.
The memo’s deficiencies make more sense once you understand that it’s not a real attempt to grapple with facts or reality, but is really just an expression of Damore’s feelings — a reactionary flailing to justify a broken status quo.
The numbers are stark: there are far more male technical workers in Silicon Valley than there are female technical workers. The National Center for Women and Information Technology places the percentage of women in “professional computing professions” at 26 percent in 2016. In this respect, Google is actually doing much worse than the rest of the industry at large: only 20 percent of its technical employees are women. (Microsoft reports approximately 18 percent, Apple reports 23 percent, and Facebook 18 percent.)
It’s not like gender inequality permeates Silicon Valley all the way through. When counting non-technical workers, companies are much closer to gender parity: Google employs 48 percent women, Microsoft with 39 percent, Apple has 38 percent, and Facebook 60 percent.
But the gender skew tends to get worse in the more powerful levels of the industry. A 2014 report found that only 11 percent of the top 150 Silicon Valley companies have at least one woman in an executive position. In 2017, Crunchbase found that only 17 percent of startups had at least one female founder, and that percentage hasn’t grown since 2012.
Meanwhile, only 6 percent of partners in venture capital firms are women, which is down from 11 percent in 1999. This is a downturn with consequences since VC firms with women partners are more likely to invest in companies founded by women.
Women currently represent about 20 percent of bachelor’s degrees in computer science — about the same proportion as women technical workers at Google. That’s all well and good, until you realize that, in 1985, women represented 37 percent of computer science bachelor’s degrees.
Today, women earn more than half of all undergraduate degrees. Within just STEM itself, women earn the majority of bachelor’s degrees in biology, half the bachelor’s degrees in chemistry, and a little under half the bachelor’s degrees in math.
These declines run counter to other trends. Not only are more women going to college and majoring in STEM fields than ever before, but girls are also keeping pace with boys in high school, earning the same number of credits in math and science and even earning slightly higher grades in those subjects. There’s still an achievement gap when it comes to high-stakes tests, like the math portion of the SAT, but even that has shrunk dramatically in the last few decades.
In the 1980s, high-achieving boys on the math portion of the SAT (scoring 700 or above) outnumbered girls 13:1. Now the ratio has diminished to 2.8:1. Something about American math and science education has changed in the last few decades, and the result is more women in STEM than ever before. This is true except when it comes to tech, where female representation is somehow decreasing instead of increasing.
Damore’s memo assumes that the case for diversity is inherently borne of left-wing values, and is therefore somehow suspect. It’s a fallacious assumption that truth lies somewhere in the middle of two poles. But beyond that, it misunderstands the corporate imperative for diversity. It’s a veneer of good public relations, for sure, but it’s also a crass question of money and the American economic advantage in a global economy.
Some projections indicate that the tech industry is about to experience a labor shortage, and it’s not clear where the needed supply of skilled workers is going to come from. Meanwhile, attrition for women is much higher than it is for men in STEM fields — and much more pronouncedly so when it comes to tech. Forty-one percent of women leave tech companies after 10 years of experience, as opposed to 17 percent of men who leave.
“Reducing female attrition by one-quarter would add 220,000 people to the highly qualified [science, engineering, and technology] labor pool,” says a 2008 Harvard Business Review research report.
The technology industry is bleeding experienced workers in a time when labor — particularly experienced labor — is more needed than ever. The primary reason for midcareer attrition is that women feel stalled in their careers: they are promoted too slowly or aren’t able to navigate a transition into management.
The absence of women in technology is a self-perpetuating problem
Part of the reason is that women in technology aren’t able to tap into the same social connections that men can. Some studies have labeled this “good old boy culture” as shorthand; other studies have simply called it “unfairness.” At its most benign, “good old boy culture” will merely slow a woman’s career trajectory, but at its worst, women experience intimidation, hostility, and harassment.
At some point, the absence of women in technology becomes a self-perpetuating problem. Because women leave, there are few senior women. Without preexisting networks of senior women to mentor junior women, careers stall at a critical midpoint, and the women leave. When looking in from the outside, many young women see a field dominated by men and choose not to enter at all to begin with.
It’s not like good old boy culture doesn’t exist in other fields and industries. Some unfortunate facts about Western history that James Damore neglects to mention (e.g., women not being considered people) have made it so that women have had no choice but to spend the last century or so carving out spaces for themselves in all kinds of male-dominated professions. But the representation of women in tech is dire even compared to science and engineering fields. There is something about tech that’s even worse.
the representation of women in tech is dire compared to science and engineering
Is the stereotype of computer programming as a masculine activity having a negative effect on women in the industry? Could implicit bias affect women’s performance evaluations and slow their careers over time? Are people like James Damore costing the technology industry $16 billion a year in turnover and attrition? These are possibilities that may be deeply uncomfortable to consider. Inequality is a big systemic problem that, by definition, some people unfairly benefit from. We would all rather be able to attribute our success to our personal characteristics, and wrapping your head around how systems are unintentionally rigged to perpetuate unfairness can be a little taxing.
The numbers point to something cultural about the tech industry that not only keeps women from entering but also drives away the women who manage to join anyway. This is a culture that Kara Swisher describes as “ruled by the equivalent of badly-raised boys who eschewed the kind of discipline and rigor that is the real requirement of success.” That’s the best way to make sense of the numbers, the trends, and the attrition — and it’s the hypothesis that fits with the anecdotes that engineers like Susan Fowler have published about toxic cultures at companies like Uber.
there’s something cultural about the tech industry that keeps women from entering and then drives them away
All of this adds up to a perfectly good explanation for the bizarre gender skew in Silicon Valley. It might be a personally discomfiting one to some, but that’s not a good reason to dismiss the long history of women contributing to tech and instead turn to bad science. “It’s almost strange to have to rationally refute it because it is just so wrong,” says tech historian Marie Hicks, a professor at the University of Wisconsin-Madison and author of the book Programmed Inequality: How Britain Discarded Women Technologists and Lost Its Edge in Computing
According to Damore, women are — on average — more neurotic, more interested in people rather than things, more agreeable, more cooperative and therefore less competitive, less assertive, and less motivated to lead. These traits, which he attributes in part to biological differences, are why women choose not to enter stressful but lucrative careers in tech, or push for promotions. Damore has said that he’s not claiming women aren’t suited for programming — just that, for biological reasons, they’re not interested. But behind this supposedly gracious claim, a lot of his arguments do imply that women just don’t have the right qualities to succeed.
If Damore had taken a closer look at the scientific literature, he would have seen that it’s not at all clear that men and women’s brains or behaviors are different enough on average to claim one sex or gender is better suited to tech or leadership than the other. If he went even further, he’d see that there’s very little scientists know for certain about which behaviors are due to biology, and which are because of society’s expectations of both men and women. In fact, Damore ignores (or resists) the complexity of the terms sex (generally considered biological) and gender (generally considered socially constructed).
Even a researcher whom Damore cites to back up his assertions disagrees with how the software engineer interpreted the data, Wired reports. David Schmitt at Bradley University, whose study found that women report more neuroticism, extraversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness in most of the 55 countries surveyed, told Wired that the differences in neuroticism just weren’t that big. “It is unclear to me that this sex difference would play a role in success within the Google workplace (in particular, not being able to handle stresses of leadership in the workplace. That’s a huge stretch to me.)”
In fact, there’s a huge body of work that says men and women, on average, tend to think and act a lot alike. “He ignores massive evidence of gender similarities in many psychological attributes that are relevant to tech jobs,” Janet Hyde, a professor of psychology at the University of Wisconsin-Madison, said in an email to The Verge.
Hyde would know. For a 2005 paper, she reviewed 46 studies called meta-analyses, which summarize the findings from lots of previous research. The idea behind this kind of study is that any weird results will vanish, helping scientists estimate the size of an effect. She found few gender differences in math skills, leadership ability, and a preference for powerful jobs. In fact, overall, she reported that 78 percent of gender differences studied over the years turned out to be small to nonexistent.
Subsequent research, like an independent meta-analysis led by Ethan Zell at the University of North Carolina at Greensboro, backs up Hyde’s findings. Another reports that women are actually significantly more effective leaders than men, even though men are more confident in their own abilities.
Of course, there are also a handful of differences that keep cropping up — exceptions to the rule. “Men are physically stronger and more physically aggressive, masturbate more, and are more positive on casual sex,” Wharton professor Adam Grant writes in a post on LinkedIn. “So you can make a case for having more men than women… if you’re fielding a sports team or collecting semen.” The tech industry, one hopes, is doing neither.
Women powered the tech sector during and after World War II, using the codebreaking computer called the Colossus in the UK. Women put together, operated, and troubleshot work on these computers. After the war, the Colossi were destroyed, but women continued to be the main tech workforce. In the US, a team of women programmed the World War II-era ENIAC, or Electronic Numerical Integrator and Computer, although their contributions received little recognition for almost 50 years.
At first, men didn’t want in on computing
At the time, programming was low paid, not prestigious, and not considered particularly intellectual. But the challenging work didn’t deserve that reputation: Jean Jennings Bartik, one of the ENIAC’s original programmers, said the machine was a “son of a bitch” to program. “It took years and her autobiography coming out for us to know who really did the work because her male managers took credit for it,” Hicks told The Verge in an email.
At first, men didn’t want in on computing. Hicks reports that in 1955, the UK’s Aeronautical Research Department said, “Boys generally prefer laboratory work to computing… this might be due in part to the absence of any recognized career in computing and of any suitable specialist courses.”
Then the power of computing started to become clear — and suddenly this low-paid women’s grunt work started to look appealing to men. The women who had written and tested some of the earliest computer programs were pushed into sales, demonstration, or assistant roles, Hicks writes. Men began taking over the industry — hired into managerial positions because of their people skills. “They’re doing the same work as the men who are replacing them with better salaries and better titles — and the men have less experience,” Hicks says. “It’s crazy-making to say that women aren’t good at technical pursuits, or that there’s any reason other than structural inequality that’s making women stay away from computing work.”
The cultural idea of a programmer went from the ENIAC girl to a beardy weirdo in sandals
In the 1960s, big employers like the System Development Corporation (SDC) and IBM began to rely heavily on personality profiles and aptitude tests to hire programmers, writes Nathan Ensmenger, a professor at Indiana University. The personality profiles of successful programmers, created by psychologists starting in the late 1960s, codified many of the stereotypes that abound today, like how “programmers dislike activities involving close personal interaction. They prefer to work with things rather than people.”
By this time, the world of computers no longer seemed to belong to the ENIAC girls. The myth of the male programmer that we know all too well was already full-fledged and seeping into the cultural consciousness. In 1968, a respected industry analyst described the programmer as “often egocentric, slightly neurotic, and he borders upon a limited schizophrenia.” (Oddly, in 1968, being neurotic made men better programmers.) “The incidence of beards, sandals, and other symptoms of rugged individualism or nonconformity are notably greater among this demographic group. Stories about programmers and their attitudes and peculiarities are legion, and do not bear repeating here,” he went on to say.
The cultural idea of a programmer went from the ENIAC girl to a beardy weirdo in sandals, and from there created a self-fulfilling prophecy and feedback loop between culture and industry that has persisted into the present day. It was helped along by the industry’s heavy reliance on aptitude tests and personality profiles, which, Ensmenger writes, encoded a functional bias against women. It wasn’t an intentional attempt to lock women out of the industry; it was merely the result of laziness. “There was widespread evidence, even in the late 1960s, that psychometric testing was inaccurate, unscientific, had been widely compromised, and was a poor predictor of future performance. Nevertheless, these methods continued to be used simply because they were convenient.”
As men began replacing women, pay in the tech industry rose
Men may not have been out to drive women from the computing industry, but no one stopped it from happening, either. And as men began replacing women, pay in the tech industry rose. When women started joining male-dominated fields like design or biology, pay dropped. There’s a signal there: women’s work isn’t valued as highly because of gender bias. After all, if a girl can do it, “It just doesn’t look like it’s as important to the bottom line or requires as much skill,” Paula England, a sociology professor at New York University, told The New York Times. “Gender bias sneaks into those decisions.”
As Damore says, everyone has biases. And most people have good intentions. Which is why it’s so important that industries have strategies for reducing bias. When symphony orchestras started conducting blind auditions, for example, women began being hired more frequently — not out of any sort of favoritism, but because the screens prevented bias from tainting the hiring process. “Blind auditions may account for 25 percent of the increase in the percentage of orchestra musicians who are female,” write the authors of the study, economists from Harvard and Princeton.
“He frames this whole thing as if it’s about intellectual biases but he misses completely, omits, ignores, analyses of actual power and discrimination — who has been closed out systematically?” says Rebecca Jordan-Young, professor of Women’s, Gender, and Sexuality studies at Barnard College.
There are studies that support Damore’s assertion that women, on average, prefer working with people and men prefer working with things, like a meta-analysis published in 2009 that analyzed career-interest surveys conducted since the 1970s. But the researchers who did this study, led by Rong Su at the University of Iowa, also found that the way these interest inventories were constructed could exacerbate or minimize those differences.
Overall, women were generally more different from each other than they were from men. “Within-gender differences are almost always much larger than between-gender differences,” Su told The Verge in an email. “Meaning that there are plenty of women who are more things-oriented than an average man and there are plenty of men who are more people-oriented than an average woman.” Su and her colleagues also argue people’s interests are also heavily shaped by culture.
That’s where the hoary old argument about “innate” biological differences runs into trouble. It obviously overlooks the biggest confounding effect of all: culture. Any innate psychological differences that may or may not exist between the sexes at birth are then shaped by the things other people say and do, the way we are treated by our peers, parents, and teachers — and how we see them being treated, psychologists David Miller and Diane Halpern write in a 2013 review of the literature.
So it’s next to impossible to separate biological influences from cultural influences. What’s more, Miller and Halpern write, “Broad cultural factors such as gender equity can even reverse these sex differences in particular contexts and nations.” So, if women were hired, promoted, and supported in the tech world, women’s preference for non-tech careers that Damore points to might well disappear.
The idea that men and women are profoundly different is familiar and easy to understand, says Rebecca Jordan-Young, a professor at Barnard College and author of the book Brain Storm: The Flaws in the Science of Sex Differences. “A lot of the critiques that we’re putting out require a little more thinking, and they’re more complex.”
Damore attempts to argue against the profound influence of culture by claiming, “These differences are not socially constructed because … [t]hey’re universal across human cultures.” And it’s true. Schmitt’s work shows sex differences in personality across 55 nations. Richard Lippa, a psychology professor at California State University, Fullerton, whom Damore also cites, came to similar conclusions after analyzing the results of a BBC internet survey from 200,000 people in 53 countries.
But Kenneth Leonard, an economics professor at the University of Maryland, comes to the exact opposite conclusion. He studied competitiveness in women and girls in two isolated, and wildly different cultures: the patriarchal Maasai tribe of Tanzania, and the matrilineal Khasi tribe in India. He and his colleagues found that Maasai women were less competitive than the men, but the matrilineal Khasi women were more competitive. So Damore’s point that the universality of “female” personality traits means they must be biological doesn’t hold up — at least, for the cooperativity / competitiveness dichotomy he poses. “It cannot be true because we can show that women behave differently according to culture,” Leonard told The Verge.
It feels as though every few years we are doomed to rehash the same controversy, and every iteration seems to get clunkier. Murray and Herrnstein wrote a book; Larry Summers gave an unscholarly conference talk; Tim Hunt talked about the “trouble with girls” in research labs; and James Damore passed around a Google Doc full of Wikipedia links. “They’re not new ideas, there’s nothing new about trying to push back against progressive hiring and anti-discrimination policies,” Jordan-Young says.
Damore’s own attempt at this classic maneuver is shot through with a number of fallacies, the biggest of which is the structural fallacy of ignoring history while assuming that the burden of proof rests with diversity advocates to show that inequality is the result of discrimination rather than the inevitable consequence of biology. Women in the US have had the right to vote for less than a hundred years. There are some pretty good reasons why inequality persists in 2017 — big structural issues that are real and clearly linked with education, opportunity, and advancement. Why turn to bad biology when you can look at history?
Leaders don’t lead things; they lead people
And the biological differences that Damore cites aren’t even logically connected to success in computer programming. For example, he says that men are more interested in things than people, but then goes on to suggest that this makes men better suited to tech or “leadership.” But leaders don’t lead things, they lead people. And for that matter, “tech” isn’t entirely thing-oriented, either.
“Essentially, engineering is all about cooperation, collaboration, and empathy for both your colleagues and your customers,” writes Yonatan Zunger, formerly of Google. “If someone told you that engineering was a field where you could get away with not dealing with people or feelings, then I’m very sorry to tell you that you have been lied to. Solitary work is something that only happens at the most junior levels, and even then it’s only possible because someone senior to you — most likely your manager — has been putting in long hours to build up the social structures in your group that let you focus on code.”
Let’s set that aside for just a moment, however, and assume that doing “tech” means you don’t have to deal with people or feelings. Is Damore arguing that men should form a massive underclass of drone-like, thing-oriented engineers managed by a superior overclass of emotionally intelligent women? Of course not. That would be absurd.
But it’s only absurd because it’s not the way things actually are. The memo isn’t reaching for a higher truth — it is instead the expression of a reactionary instinct to preserve the status quo. Deflection: now, with graphs!
Meritocracy is an ideal, not a reality
It’s tempting to believe that when we’re successful, our successes are solely because of our own merits and that everyone ends up succeeding to the extent they deserve. But meritocracy is an ideal, not a reality. “It’s something we wish was real, in our better moments we aspire to it,” Hicks says. “But it’s dangerous to think about it as a thing that operates.” It’s a simplistic view of the world that’s only comforting to the people on top.
“There’s currently very little transparency into the extent of our diversity programs which keeps it immune to criticism from those outside its ideological echo chamber,” Damore writes in his memo. He goes on to say, “We haven’t been able to measure any effect of our Unconscious Bias training and it has the potential for overcorrecting or backlash, especially if made mandatory.” Without providing any details or support or citations — only speculation and what may even be his own emotional reaction to having undergone Unconscious Bias training — Damore argues that Google’s diversity initiatives have not been adequately considered or rigorously vetted.
But the actual diversity programs at Google are fairly milquetoast. A spokesperson for Google pointed to things like a six-week increase in maternity leave (from 12 weeks to 18), active recruiting of women candidates, internships for women, and a summer camp for graduating high school seniors. These measures, and what we know about Google’s Unconscious Bias training, are all in line with what various reports and studies have recommended to address diversity in the technology industry.
How can you address a problem if it remains invisible?
Training to eliminate or mitigate bias, in particular, is key. It’s the foundation on which everything else rests. How can you address a problem if it remains invisible? To that end, the industry has poured money into numerous studies and projects to suss out what went wrong and how to fix it. Case in point: Google’s own “Women Who Choose Computer Science” study.
In that sense, Damore’s memo represents a huge step backward. It’s an arrogant and sloppy cherry-picking of assertions in order to argue against Google’s Unconscious Bias training, a program intended to remedy inequity by educating people. The memo cloaks itself in a pretense of rationality but is actually an artifact of willful, cultivated ignorance that no thinking person can take seriously.
And yet it’s a surefire bet that this exact same controversy will rear its head again in the very near future. Wherever there is inequality, people will invoke bad science to justify doing nothing about it. But with any luck, with each successive rehash, more and more people will understand this argument for what it really is: a whole lot of feelings.
]]>Senate Majority Leader Mitch McConnell is pushing a new bill that would block all tobacco and vape purchases for Americans under 21 years old, according to an announcement today that was reported by Bloomberg.
In a statement today, McConnell presented the bill as a response to widespread public health risks posed by teen vaping. “For some time, I’ve been hearing from the parents who are seeing an unprecedented spike in vaping among their teenage children,” McConnell said. “In addition, we all know people who started smoking at a young age and who struggled to quit as adults. Unfortunately it’s reaching epidemic levels around the country.” McConnell says he will look to the 11 states that already have Tobacco 21 laws on the books for ideas.
But vaping companies don’t seem concerned. Juul, which sold a 35 percent stake to tobacco giant Altria for $12.8 billion last year, applauded McConnell for today’s announcement. “JUUL Labs is committed to eliminating combustible cigarettes, the number one cause of preventable death in the world and to accomplish that goal, we must restrict youth usage of vapor products,” Juul’s CEO Kevin Burns said in an emailed statement. “Tobacco 21 laws fight one of the largest contributors to this problem – sharing by legal-age peers – and they have been shown to dramatically reduce youth usage rates.”
That support might have to do with Juul’s issues with the Food and Drug Administration. Over the past year, Juul has come under the FDA’s fire for its massive popularity among young people. So supporting a higher minimum age could help its image and take some of the regulatory pressure off. From an industry perspective, the move is fairly low risk since the product is already embedded in the population, and people under age 21 may already be addicted, says Kathleen Hoke, a law professor at the University of Maryland. “We can change this age to 21 but we’re going to have to work extraordinarily hard at the state and local level to actually get cigarettes or vape products or chew out of the hands of the 18 to 20 year olds,” she says.
At least on the surface, raising the minimum legal age for smoking aligns McConnell with public health advocates who have been pushing for raising the smoking (and vaping) age to 21. The goal is to keep kids from starting a lifelong nicotine addiction — since some 90 percent of current smokers took their first drags on a cigarette by age 18, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention say. A 2015 report from the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine projected there would be a 12 percent drop in smoking by the year 2100 if the legal age for buying tobacco products were increased immediately.
But the bill’s success will depend on how it’s crafted. Rob Crane, professor of family medicine at The Ohio State University and president of the Preventing Tobacco Addiction Foundation, is skeptical that it will really hold tobacco retailers responsible for selling to people who are underage. From the more than 450 cities and counties that have passed Tobacco 21 laws, “what we have found that does work is when you make local health departments under civil law do the enforcement,” he says. “For a rogue retailer that keeps on selling, there’s a risk of license suspension.”
But if the law winds up penalizing convenience store clerks who sell vapes and tobacco products to kids, the retailer who’s profiting gets off scot-free, he says. In the end, Crane is skeptical of the motivations behind the bill, no matter what form it takes. “This is all a PR move to keep Juul out of the hot seat from the FDA.”
]]>